There are very few areas of science in
which a single variable thing leads to another with significance consequences
unless, that is, one is a mechanistic scientist who is not interested in
complexity or subtlety.
Take the simple act of remembering something.
We remember people, events, places, things. But we don’t know how. All attempts
to “locate” memory through fMRI scans and other brain imaging techniques have
failed and memory also seems to be something that can be shared. Experiments
with rats who learn to run a water filled maze show that, once several hundred
rats have learned to “swim the maze”, rats who were not part of the experiment
can develop this ability faster, with some seeming to know “instinctively” how
to run the same maze. Materialist and mechanistic biologists are continuing to
look for the “memory” location in the brain, but others have moved on and have
developed what is known as the “morphic resonance” theory of memory in which
memories are available in the unconscious and can be shared within cultures and
between people without any physical connection between these people. It may
sound far fetched, but the evidence for this theory is beginning to be
commanding.
So when we see a simple correlation, like
CO2 = climate change, we need to be suspicious. There is no doubt that
greenhouse gasses are part of the processes involved in the constantly changing
climate, but it would be unusual to say the least for this to be such a
dominant factor. We need also to consider
the role of the sun, ocean heat uptake, ocean circulation, water vapour, sea
ice, land topography, distance from the equator, cloud formation, and el NiƱo
and la Nina (the El Nino southern
oscillation cycle). There are also feedback mechanisms in place which affect
the interaction of these factors and impact weather and climate.
Weather is thus complex and climate is the
sum of weather activities over time. There is no simple relationship between
CO2 and climate change. Further, climate is unpredictable. We cannot predict weather
much more than 5-7 weeks ahead, but we seem to be confident that we can see
what the climate patterns one hundred and two hundred years from now will be
using computer simulations based on our understanding of climate systems. None
of these models have been able to accurately predict past climate patterns, and
none came close to predicting that there has been no increase in global surface
temperature for over seventeen years. The “error rate” when model predictions
are compared to actual observations from weather stations and satellites
suggest that the models are poor predictors, but getting better. It is, after
all, early days in the study of climate.
So why do many want to believe that there
is such a simple man-made explanation of climate change? What is the psychology
of this belief system?
Those who support the anthropogenic theory
of climate change – e.g. mankind is responsible and therefore mankind can act
to change climate – are locking into three belief systems. The first is the
belief in the power of experts, in this case climate scientists and economists.
The second is the age-old belief that mankind can change natural patterns and
nature. The third is that climate change as a belief system has replaced
religion for some.
It’s
About Science
Mankind has long had a faith in
science. Such faith is based on an
understanding of science as impartial, truth-seeking and independent of
political, social or economic influence. This is known in the philosophy of science
as “the immaculate conception of science” – science as truth. This is not,
however, how science is practiced. Scientists are influenced in just the same way
as are politicians, businessmen and women and advocates for a cause. Science is
linked to economics by the rent-seeking behaviour of scientists who use grants
and donations to fund their work, with these grants and donations reflecting a
view of what is worth pursuing. Science is linked to society by a sense of
“social good”. When society wished to “deal with deviance”, science obliged
with the evidence to support eugenics (the practice of involuntary
sterilization of those who were deemed mentally deficient, which continued
until the early 1970’s); when governments wished to criminalize drugs, evidence
was adduced by scientists which showed the pathway from marijuana or cannabis
to hard drugs like heroin was clear and firm (the gateway drug theory) – a
pathway now largely discredited.
Science is not “pure”, it is influenced by
policy and investment choices – it is a socially conditioned activity. This has
been the debate within the philosophy of science for some time – the debate
between two schools of thought. One sees science as divorced from politics and
economics and social influence (Popper) and the other sees science as embedded
in social and economic considerations (Polyani). Indeed, revolutionary thinking
in the philosophy of science suggests that evidence be used to campaign for
social and political change since scientists should be advocates for social
good (Feyerabend).
While many in science try to maintain a
scientific independence, the reality is that this is very difficult to do
within the science establishment. For example, challenging an orthodox theory
of intelligence or personality or supporting research into psychic phenomenon
can be difficult to do when the “norms” of psychology as a science require a
certain degree of compliance to a dominant view. Recent events related to the esteemed
climatologist, Professor Lennart Bengtsson,
who chose to disagree with the dominant anthropogenic view of climate
change and to do so by joining the scientific advisory board of the Global
Warming Policy Foundation, earning the wrath of many on the “other side” of
this debate as well as death threats., demonstrate this. Richard Tol, a lead
author for the International Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) has suggested that
competent people who take a view contrary to that of their own government on
climate change are not invited to participate in the IPCC process, again making
clear that social and political considerations are part of scientific activity.
One final example, researchers within the Polar Bear Specialist Group (PBSG) of
the IUCN have admitted that the “best guess” of how many polar bears were in
the Arctic was in part based on meeting social expectations that the number
should be low, enabling politicians to declare polar bears an endangered
species. Polar bear numbers continue to grow for most, but not all, polar bear
populations.
Some have suggested that critics of anthropogenic
climate change theory are funded by the interests of coal and energy
conglomerates. Indeed, Greenpeace and others are now seeking to establish the
legal basis for a court case in which energy companies are to be accused of
manipulating science to support their claims that CO2 is not the primary cause
of global climate change. What Greenpeace is not doing is asking if the
billions spent by Governments and others around the world in support of the
anthropogenic view of climate change has an equal impact on science. Surely, if
who makes the investment influences the outcome this would apply to any
investor. The idea that governments are neutral is somewhat laughable.
It is often suggested that 97% of climate
scientists support the anthropogenic view of climate change. This is based on
political statements made by non-climate experts which in turn are based on
some very problematic studies which seek to establish where scientists “sit” on
this issue. A recent Wall Street Journal article as well as testimony before
the US Congress shows just how flawed this claim is. Further, science is not
about majority votes: it is about evidence and theory. New evidence can change
theory and theories change over time as evidence becomes better understood. If
a majority of scientists believe something, does this make it right? In 1982, Stanley B. Prusiner of
the University of California, San Francisco announced that his team
had purified the hypothetical infectious prion, and that the infectious agent
consisted mainly of a specific protein which in turn was the cause of
scrapie in sheep and CJD in humans. Prusiner won the Nobel prize for his
discovery. In a recent interview he made clear that 97% of scientists working
in his field rejected his ideas when first announced and that, even after the
Nobel prize, he puts the current figure at 50%. Science has a culture in which
skepticism is encouraged at one level and frowned upon when it looks like it is
showing evidence of being a serious challenge to orthodoxy.
Faith in Experts
A part of the belief
in science is also a belief in scientists as experts. We know a lot about
experts and how poor they are at expert prediction.
Indeed, the so-called expert
“consensus” position in climate science is based on selective use of evidence,
some of it from peer reviewed journals and some not, and expert group-think.
Psychologists understand this phenomenon and have developed a thorough
understanding of just how wrong experts can be.
Phillip Tetlock author of Expert
Political Judgement and a Professor of Psychology at Penn State
University, provides strong
empirical evidence for just how bad they are at predicting events. He conducted
a long-running experiment that asked nearly 300 political experts to make a
variety of forecasts about dozens of countries around the world. After tracking
the accuracy of about 80,000 predictions over the course of 20 years, Tetlock
found:
That experts
thought they knew more than they knew. That there was a systematic gap between
subjective probabilities that experts were assigning to possible futures and
the objective likelihoods of those futures materializing … With respect to
how they did relative to, say, a baseline group of Berkeley undergraduates
making predictions, they did somewhat better than that. How did they do
relative to purely random guessing strategy? Well, they did a little bit better
than that, but not as much as you might hope …
The psychologist
Daniel Kahneman, who won the Nobel Prize in Economics for his work on
decision-making, has looked at the issue of “experts” and why they so often get
things wrong. In his book Thinking Fast and Slow he
points to several aspects of their psychology as factors, but highlights two in
particular: the illusion of understanding and the illusion of validity. These
are primary causes of experts getting it wrong.
The illusion of
understanding refers to the idea that the world is more knowable than it
actually is. In particular, experts believe that they have an in-depth and
insightful understanding of the past and this enables them to better understand
the future. They use what Kahneman refers to as the WYSIATI rule –
“what you see is all that there is” and this provides the basis for their
confidence.
For example, it must
be the case that high levels of government indebtedness (levels of
debt to GDP ratio above 90% is the most recent version of this[3]) stifle the
economy and reduce investor and entrepreneurial confidence according to some
notable economists. Or it is obvious that human generated C02
is the major cause of climate change according to some climatologists. Both of
these understandings are based on a particular view of historical data and
“facts” and an extrapolation of these views into the future.
The views exist
independently of the evidence to support them. Just as
financial advisers are confident that they are successful in
predicting the future behaviour of stocks, so macro-economists are
confident that their views of austerity have the weight of history behind them.
Those committed to the view that human produced CO2 is the primary cause of
climate change are not deterred by evidence that it may not be or that climate
change has stalled for the last seventeen years.
Experts are sustained
in their beliefs by a professional culture that supports them. Austerians
(those who believe that austerity is the only way) have their own network
of support, as do the Keynesians who oppose them. Anthroprocene climatologists
who believe that man is the primary cause of global warming have their own
network of support among climate change researchers and politicians
while the skeptical climate scientists also have their support networks. All
remain ignorant of their ignorance and are sustained in their belief systems by
selected use of evidence and by the support of stalwarts. These supportive
networks and environments help sustain the illusion of validity. It is an
illusion because evidence which demonstrates contrary views to those of the
“experts” are dismissed and denied – the expert position, whatever it may be,
is valid simply because they are expert.
Indeed, using Isaiah
Berlin’s 1953 work on Tolstoy (The Hedgehog and the Fox), Austerians and
anthropocenes are “hedgehogs” – they know one big thing, they know what they
know within a coherent framework, they bristle with impatience towards those
who don’t see things their way and they are exceptionally focused on their
forecasts. For these experts a “failed prediction” is an issue of timing, the
kind of evidence being adduced and so on – it is never due to the fact that
their prediction is wrong. Austerians who look at the failure of their policies
in Europe, for example, suggest that the austerity did not go far enough;
anthroprocene climatologists see the lack of warming over the last seventeen
years as proof that they are right, it is just that the timing is a little out.
Even the climatologist trapped in thick ice in the Antarctic in December 2013
who set out to study the thinning ice-cap claims he just went to the wrong
place – “climate change is happening and the ice is melting” he says, as he is
lifted off the thick ice by helicopter.
Tetlock’s work, cited
above, is a powerful testimony to these two illusions – understanding and
validity. His results are devastating for the notion of “the expert”. According
to Kahneman, “people who spend their time, and earn their living, studying
a particular topic produce poorer predictions than dart throwing monkeys”.
Tetlock observes that
“experts in demand were more overconfident that those who eked our
existences far from the limelight”. We can see this in spades in both economics
and climate change. James Hanson, recently retired from NASA and seen to be one
of the worlds leading anthroprocene climatologists, makes predictions and
claims that cannot be supported by the evidence he himself collected and was
responsible for. For example, he suggested that “in the last decade it's
warmed only about a tenth of a degree as compared to about two tenths of a
degree in the preceding decade” – a claim not supported by the data set which
he was responsible for. This overconfidence and arrogance comes from being
regarded as one of the leading climate scientists in the world – evidence is
not as important as the claim or the person making it. Hanson suffers from the
illusion of skill.
Kahneman recognizes
people like Hansen. He suggests
“…overconfident
professionals sincerely believe they have expertise, act as experts and look
like experts. You will have to struggle to remind yourself that they may be in
the grip of an illusion.”
There are other
psychological features of the expert that are worthy of reflection. For
example, how “group think” starts to permeate a discipline such that those
outside the group cannot be heard as rational or meaningful – they are referred
to as “deniers” or “outsiders”, reflecting the power of group think. The power
of a group (they will claim consensus as if this ends scientific debate) to
close ranks and limit the scope of conversation or act as gatekeepers for the
conversation. Irving Janis documented the characteristics of group think in his
1982 study of policy disasters and fiascoes[4]. He suggests
these features:
- Illusion of invulnerability –Creates excessive optimism that
encourages taking extreme risks. We can see this in the relentless pursuit
of austerity throughout Europe.
- Collective rationalization – Members discount warnings and do
not reconsider their assumptions. We see this in relation to both climate
change and austerity economics.
- Belief in inherent morality – Members believe in the rightness
of their cause and therefore ignore the ethical or moral consequences of
their decisions. Austerians appear to willfully ignore the level of
unemployment and the idea of a lost generation of youth workers,
especially in Greece and Spain. Anthropecene climate researchers generally
present themselves as morally superior.
- Stereotyped views of out-groups – Negative views of “enemy” make
effective responses to conflict seem unnecessary. Climate “deniers”
commonly face suggestions that they be prosecuted or punished in some way[5].
- Direct pressure on dissenters – Members are under pressure not to
express arguments against any of the group’s views. This has occurred in
climate change research community, since grants appear to favour those who
adopt the view that man made CO2 is the primary cause of climate change.
- Self-censorship – Doubts and deviations from the
perceived group consensus are not expressed.
- Illusion of unanimity – The majority view and judgments
are assumed to be unanimous. This is especially the case in “consensus” (sic)
climate change science and amongst austerians.
- Self-appointed ‘mindguards’ – Members protect the group and the
leader from information that is problematic or contradictory to the
group’s cohesiveness, view, and/or decisions.
- all of these
characteristics can be seen to be in play in the two examples used throughout
this chapter – economics of austerity and made man global warming.
There is also the
issue of the focusing illusion. Kahneman sums this up in a single
statement: “nothing in life is as important as you think it is when you are
thinking about it”. “Government debt is the most important economic challenge
facing society today” says a well known economists, or “climate change is a
life and death issue” says US Secretary of State, John Kerry. Neither of
these statements are true for anyone unless they are
obsessive.
Climate Change as Religion
“Because
it is what science tells us” is one explanation for why some people believe in
the anthropogenic view of climate change, dutifully ignoring the growing
evidence that this theory about climate (CO2 = climate change) is weak. Many people
look up to expert opinion and permit this to influence their own thinking. But
the other reason is that they want to believe because it means we can act in
the good of humanity: climate change is religion, albeit in a very particular
sense.
Indeed, in 2009 Mr. Justice Michael Burton,
ruling on an employment appeal matter in the British legal system, ruled that a
sincerely held belief in climate change and the need to act accordingly
constituted a belief system according to the Employment Equity (Religion and
Belief) Regulations of 2003. That is, climate change was not a new religion but
had equivalent status under the law. Those who “believe” need the protection of
the law to enable their beliefs.
All religions have a certain
characteristics. Niels Nelson, in his book Religions
of the World, suggests twelve such characteristics. Amongst these two are
these three:
- 1. Providing a coherent worldview.
- 2. Promoting social organization and collective action.
- 3. Offering future hope, provided certain actions are taken
Environmentalists have developed a coherent
worldview – e.g. the planet is endangered by mankind and we should respect
nature and return the planet to its “natural state” (sic) and not damage it
further through overpopulation, deforestation, industrialization, globalization
and the emission of CO2 – around which many feel able to organize some of the
whole or their lives. Future hope is provided by the simple idea that we can
“stop” climate change by massively reducing CO2, even though this may mean
radically changing the nature of developed society.
So profound are the belief systems
associated with this particular world-view, that those who do not share this
view are vilified. There have been calls for climate change “deniers” to be
criminalized, prosecuted for crimes against humanity (a call made by James Hansen formerly of
NASA), with some calling for public trials and executions – all of which are
contained in a 2007 US Senate Report. Many of the more rabid suggestions about
what should be done with those who are sceptical about anthropogenic causes of
climate change rival what was done in the Spanish Inquisition – indeed, some
would make the inquisitors blush.
So why the fervour? Some environmentalists
sincerely believe that the planet is in peril and that it is their duty to save
it. Further, they believe that time is
running out (“the day of judgement is upon us”) to act – with estimates ranging
from days, to weeks to months. It is urgent that we act (repent) and do so with
gusto, so as to change the nature of nature.
Prince Charles is a follower of this
religion. He recently told business leaders that it was necessary to “fundamental transformation of global
capitalism” in order to halt “dangerously accelerating climate change” that
would “bring us to our own destruction”.
He went on:
“Over the next eighteen months, and bearing in mind the urgency of the
situation confronting us, the world faces what is probably the last effective
window of opportunity to vacate the insidious lure of the ‘last chance saloon’
in order to agree an ambitious, equitable and far-sighted multilateral
settlement in the context of the post-2015 sustainable development goals and
the U.N. Framework Convention on Climate Change”.
Put simply: we are
doomed now if we do not repent and engage in acts of contrition and change our
sinful ways. The end is upon us. A hypothesis which remains unproven has become
an unquestioned truth.
Conclusion
As a psychologist and part-time philosopher
of science the issue of a belief in anthropogenic theory of climate change
poses interesting challenges. So many people are so passionate about something
they know so little about and the evidence, when reviewed systematically, is
not as compelling as many claim it is. Faith, not science, and commitment, not
evidence, is driving and shaping behaviour.
Experts are no better than a group of “dart
throwing monkeys” according to Kahneman, yet experts are who we are asked to believe and many are overly
confident that experts know what they are talking about, after all “they are
scientists”. Yet science is not a search for truth but a social and
economically driven activity in which the cultural requirements for group-think
are high, especially when supported by rent-seeking and status seeking
behaviour.
Climate change is occurring, but we don’t fully
understand the complexities of climate or how the changes are occurring. The
earth is not warming, the sea level rises are within normal limits, yet the
belief that the evidence is all pointing to catastrophe remains strong. C02
continues to rise, but the global surface temperature is stable – a challenge
to the theory of anthropogenic global warming. Extreme weather events are known
not to be directly connected to CO2, but the belief that they are persists,
even amongst experts. A social psychological explanation of these beliefs and
behaviours is needed.
1 comment:
Thanks for this sophisticated essay, Stephen. I have placed a link to it in my own blog www.case4all.info under the tab Dissenting Views. It is necessary to encourage a healthy scepticism about all the "stories" or world views to which we commit. I do think that the evidence for anthropogenic climate disruption is more compelling than do your sources, but wonder if the belief in exponential economic growth could also be cast as a religion?
Post a Comment